Where The Boys Are
Christina Hoff
Sommers
A Bradley Lecture delivered at the American Enterprise
Institute
I am Christina Hoff Sommers, W.H. Brady Fellow at the American Enterprise
Institute. For most of my professional career I have been writing on ethics and
moral philosophy. I have also written extensively about the influence of
feminism on American culture. This evening, I will talk to you about some themes
in a book I am writing called The War against Boys.
I will tell you something about how boys and girls are faring educationally
and otherwise. There is a lot of misinformation on this topic. Too many advocacy
groups are shaping the discussion and painting their own alarming pictures. I
will try to give you the best and most up-to-date information on the true state
of affairs. ...
... There is a story making the rounds in education circles, about a
now-retired Chicago public school teacher, Mrs. Dougherty. Mrs. Dougherty was a
dedicated, highly respected sixth grade teacher, who could always be counted on
to bring the best out in her students.
But one year she had a class she found impossible to control. The students
were rowdy, unmanageable, and seemingly un-teachable. She began to worry that
many of them had serious learning disabilities—or mental disorders—or something.
So, one day, when the principal was out of town, she did something teachers were
forbidden to do in that school. She entered the principal’s office and looked
into the special files that listed student’s IQ’s.
To her shock she found a majority of the class was way above average in
intelligence. A large cluster was in the high 120-s –128, 127, 129; several
scored in the 130s; and one of the worst classroom culprits was in fact
brilliant. He had an IQ of 145.
Ms. Dougherty was furious. She had been feeling sorry
for those kids. Giving them remedial work. Making excuses for them. Things
were about to change.
Well, Ms. Dougherty was furious. She had been feeling sorry for those kids.
Giving them remedial work. Making excuses for them. Things were about to
change.
She went back to her class and a new era began.
She read them the riot act. They would comport themselves like ladies and
gentlemen. She doubled the homework load, raised the standards, gave draconian
punishments to any malefactor.
Slowly but perceptibly their performance began to improve. By the end of the
year, this class of ne’er-do-wells was the best behaved and highest performing
of all the sixth grade classes.
The principal was of course delighted. He knew about this class and its
reputation for incorrigibility. And one day he called her into his office and
asked her: What did you do?
She felt compelled to tell him the truth. She confessed that when he had been
out of town, she had looked up children’s IQs.
The principal forgave her. Congratulated her. Then he said something
surprising.
"I think you should know, Mrs. Dougherty - those numbers, next to the
children’s names, they’re not IQ scores, they’re their locker numbers."
I heard this story from Dr. Carl Boyd, who is president of an educational
foundation in Kansas City. He says the story is true, and I believe him. The
moral for teachers is obvious: demand and expect excellence from students and
you’ll get the best they can give. Be tough on them. Be like Mrs. Dougherty.
Some of you may think that this is self-evident, that it’s only common sense.
Who questions setting and enforcing high standards for students?
The answer is a lot of education experts.
They believe children are harmed by teachers who
enforce high standards.
Many professors and deans at our leading schools of education have convinced
themselves and others that American children are vulnerable, fragile, and in
crisis. They believe children are harmed by teachers who enforce high standards.
They want teachers to pay attention to the child’s self-esteem and emotional
stability. The fashion in many classrooms is for teachers to break the class up
into small, non-threatening, cooperative learning groups – the teacher is more
of a supportive facilitator—rather than a demanding task master. In the early
nineties it was the girls who were portrayed as being at special risk, in need
of a therapeutic pedagogy; now it’s the boys as well.
My own view is that the child-crisis is a myth. And I believe that taking a
therapeutic approach to education is a very bad idea for all students. But, as I
shall try to show, it is especially harmful to boys.
The idea that our children are fragile, being harmed by the dominant culture
which forces them into feminine or masculine gender stereotypes, is now the
fashion in education. Let me tell you who promoted and popularized this idea and
then give you my reasons for rejecting it.
The girl-crisis came first, and a single professor at Harvard University is
the person most responsible for promulgating it.
In 1989, Carol Gilligan, a professor at the Harvard School of Education and a
pioneer in the field of women’s psychology, announced her finding that the
nation’s adolescent girls were in crisis. In her words, "As the river of a
girl’s life flows into the sea of Western culture, she is in danger of drowning
or disappearing."
Gilligan believes girls are silenced
Gilligan believes girls are silenced –"they lose their voice" as they enter
adolescence in our male-centered society. Her distressing portrait of endangered
girls had no basis in reality—as I shall show. But it fascinated an uncritical
media who helped gain for it a widespread acceptance.
Soon after Gilligan issued her admonition about our drowning, silenced, and
disappearing daughters, feminist researchers and women’s advocacy groups began
reporting that the nation’s teenaged girls are academically "shortchanged,"
drained of their self-esteem by a society that favors boys.
The American Association of University Women called what was happening to
girls "an unacknowledged American tragedy." The state of the nation’s girls was
being described in increasingly lurid terms.
A Los Angeles Times writer talked of the "widespread process of psychic
suicide among ordinary teenage girls." Here is Mary Pipher in Reviving Ophelia:
Saving the Selves of Adolescent Girls (this book stayed high on the New York
Times bestseller list for more than one year). According to Pipher:
Something dramatic happens to girls in early adolescence. Just as planes
and ships disappear mysteriously into the Bermuda Triangle, so do the
selves of girls go down in droves. They crash and burn.
The allegedly low estate of America’s girls moved the United States Congress
to pass the Gender Equity Act, categorizing girls as an "under-served
population" on a par with other discriminated-against minorities.
Millions of dollars in grants were awarded to study the
plight of girls
Millions of dollars in grants were awarded to study the plight of girls and
to learn how to cope with the insidious bias against them. At the Fourth World
Conference on Women in Beijing, the American delegation presented the
educational and psychological deficits of American girls as a pressing human
rights issue.
Later in the 90’s, the crisis talk would turn to boys.
Here again Gilligan is a moving spirit. She claims to have found that boys
too are traumatized by the way they are "socialized" in what she calls the
"patriarchal social order."
She and some of her male disciples in the New England area are promoting a
movement to rescue boys from the hostile male culture that is harming them.
You may already have noticed a lot of recent stories about the crashing
"selves" of boys.
On June 4, 1998, McLean Hospital, the psychiatric teaching hospital of the
Harvard Medical School, issued a two–page press release announcing the results
of a new study on boys. The study, entitled "Listening to Boys’ Voices," was
conducted by Dr. William Pollack, Director of the Center for Men at McLean
Hospital and assistant professor of psychology at Harvard.
He says that even seemingly normal boys are "in
trouble"—they are "disconnected,"
Pollack’s conclusions are sweeping and alarming. He says that even seemingly
normal boys are "in trouble"—they are "disconnected," unable to relate to people
and unable to express emotions. Echoing the talk of girls as Ophelias, Pollack
refers to American boys as "young Hamlets [who] succumb to an inner state of
Denmark." He urges immediate action on a nationwide scale: "[A]s a nation, we
must address these boys’ pain before it reaches epidemic proportions and
severely disrupts our society."
Stories about the boy crisis appeared in a number of leading newspapers—the
New York Times, the Washington Post, the Boston Globe—the crisis made the cover
of Newsweek and was the subject of an ABC 20/20 segment; the Today show devoted
two programs to it.
Gilligan, Pipher, Pollack and their many colleagues speak of saving,
rescuing, reviving.
By using Ophelia and Hamlet as symbols, the child crisis writers offer a dark
and unwholesome portrait of America’s girls and boys. But is it accurate? Is it
helpful? Are American children well served by being portrayed as tragic and
psychologically ailing?
My answer to all three questions is an emphatic no.
The first thing to notice is that none of the crisis writers has published
their alarming findings in any peer-reviewed social science journals. Bypassing
the scrutiny of peer review both Gilligan and Pollack simply announced their
conclusions in the popular press.
More conventional scholars, who abide by the protocols of respectable social
science research, see no evidence of crisis.
Dr. Anne Peterson, a University of Minnesota, adolescent psychologist,
reports the consensus of clinicians and researchers working in adolescent
psychology:
It is now known that the majority of adolescents of both genders
successfully negotiate this developmental period without any major
psychological or emotional disorder, develop a positive sense of personal
identity, . . .and manage to forge adaptive peer relationships at the same
time they maintain close relationships with their families."
Daniel Offer, the University of Michigan Professor of Psychiatry, refers to
"a new generation of studies" that find a majority of adolescents [80%] normal
and well-adjusted."
Just consider the contrast between William Pollack’s study with its finding
that seemingly healthy boys are really distraught and desperate; and Daniel
Offer’s article with its finding that most adolescents –male or female are
psychically sound.
Offer’s article is published in a professional journal—the Journal of
American Child Adolescence. His data are available. He supports his thesis by
referencing more than 150 other peer-reviewed journal articles. He clearly
states his methodology. He carefully explains the scope and limitations of his
study.
William Pollack’s study (which I obtained by requesting it from McLean
Hospital’s public relations office) is a 30-page typed manuscript. It has never
been published and is not marked as about to be published. The manuscript
contains not a single footnote—not a single reference to other work. His
conclusions about boys are based mainly on some psychological and gender
awareness tests he administered to 150 middle school boys. We are not told how
he selected these boys, or whether they constitute anything like a
representative sample.
As far as stating its own limitations, Dr. Pollack declares, "These findings
are unprecedented in the literature of research psychology."
When Francis Crick and James Watson announced their epochal discovery of the
double helix in British journal Nature, they were calmer than Dr. Pollack.
Professor Gilligan, she has not published her
data on "drowning and disappearing" girls in social science journals
As for Professor Gilligan, she has not published her data on "drowning and
disappearing" girls in social science journals. The pronouncement that girls
were endangered was made in a book called Making Connections: The Relational
Worlds of Adolescent Girls at Emma Willard School.
Gilligan’s central thesis is that as girls enter adolescence, they lose their
"voice"–their expressiveness and confidence. What empirical basis does she have
for this claim? Gilligan and her colleagues interviewed a hundred or so boarding
school girls about how they felt growing into adolescence. Gilligan explains in
the prologue of Making Connections that the studies in the collection are not
intended as a "definitive statement about girls." Instead, "they are offered in
the spirit of celebration. . ." (p5)
Contrast Gilligan’s work with the less celebratory, but more conventional
research of Susan Harter of the University of Denver. Harter and her associates
attempted to test Gilligan’s hypothesis that girls lose their expressiveness,
their "voice," as they enter adolescence. Unlike Gilligan, Harter published her
findings in a peer reviewed professional journal, Educational Psychologist.
Gilligan interviewed 100 girls at the Emma Willard School. Harter interviewed
(approximately) 900 male and female students from grades 6-12 and from a range
of economic backgrounds and school-types. We learn little about the sort of
questions Gilligan asked the girls. Harter, on the other hand, clearly explains
her interview instrument and how it was tested for internal consistency. She
notes it limitations.
"There is no evidence in our data for a loss of
voice among female adolescents." She could not even find a trend in that
direction.
What did Harter find? "There is no evidence in our data for a loss of voice
among female adolescents." She could not even find a trend in that direction.
She and her co-investigators have now done several studies that appear to
disconfirm Gilligan’s findings. They are careful to say that these are
inconclusive and that Gilligan’s predictions about loss of voice might be true
for a subset of girls in certain domains. They suggest that more work needs to
be done. But for the time being, Harter cautions "against generalizations about
either gender as a group."
Journalists speak of Gilligan’s "landmark research" but they never ask
whether this research exists; nor do they ask her any critical questions.
What I believe is that the child-crisis writers are irresponsibly portraying
healthy American girls and boys as pathological victims of an inimical culture.
Why does the public indulge them?
So far I have been inveighing against the large, extreme, and irresponsible
claims of the crisis writers, and I suggested that they have produced no
credible evidence to back them up.
Now let me consider some of their more moderate and seemingly reasonable
assertions. Let’s consider the suggestion that boys are emotionally repressed,
out of touch with their own feelings. Is that true? And should we be concerned?
Gilligan speaks of boys as "hid[ing] their humanity" and submerging "their
very best qualities . . . their sensitivity." Are boys insensitive?
One thing is undeniable: Stereotypical boy behavior that was once considered
normal now offends and upsets a lot of people. In the fall of 1997, I took part
in a television debate with feminist lawyer Gloria Allred in which we disagreed
over male and female differences. I pointed out that younger boys and girls have
markedly different preferences and behaviors, citing the following homespun
example.
The girls dressed the dolls, talked to them, kissed
them and played house; the boys catapulted the baby carriage from the
roof.
Hasbro Toys, a major toy company, tested a play house they were considering
marketing to both boys and girls. They soon discovered that girls and boys did
not interact with the structure in the same way. The girls dressed the dolls,
talked to them, kissed them and played house; the boys catapulted the baby
carriage from the roof.
I said to Ms. Allred, "Surely you would agree, boys and girls are innately
different?"
Allred seemed to be shocked by the boy’s catapulting behavior. Apparently,
she takes it as a sign of a propensity for violence. She flatly denied there
were any innate differences. Said Allred, "If there are little boys who catapult
baby carriages off the roof of doll houses that is just one more reason why we
have to socialize boys at an earlier age, perhaps to be playing with doll
houses."
Second example of worrying and (apparently) offensive boy behavior: Ms. Logan
is a very committed middle school teacher in San Francisco who sees it as her
mission to sensitize boys, to make them more aware of how women have been
oppressed and at the same time to bring out their more loving, nurturing side.
As a class assignment she has the children make a quilt celebrating "women we
admire." But there is a problem; the boys do not always produce the kind of
muslin quilt squares the teacher wants.
There are worrying signs of persistent insensitivity.
A 12-year-old boy named Jimmy, for example, chose to honor the tennis player
Monica Seles by drawing a bloody knife on a tennis racket. It’s not the sort of
thing a girl would think of. Jimmy’s square may be unique in the history of
quilting but Ms. Logan did not appreciate its originality. She insisted he start
again and make an acceptable contribution to the class quilt.
I am afraid my own 14-year old son David provides yet another example of male
insensitivity and inadequate emotional engagement. Write Source 2000 is a widely
used middle school English textbook by Houghton-Mifflin. 1.5 million copies have
been sold—which means a lot of children use them. Like many other contemporary
reading and grammar textbooks, Write Source 2000 is chock full of exercises
designed to improve children’s self-esteem and to draw them out emotionally. My
son came to me one evening confused by his homework assignment. He asked, "Mom,
what do they want?" He had read a short story in which one character always
compared himself to another. Here were the questions David had to answer:
Do you often compare yourself with others? Do you compare to make yourself
feel better? Does your comparison ever make you feel inferior?
Another set of questions asked about profanity in the story:
- How did cursing make you feel? Do you curse? Why?
- Does cursing make you feel more powerful? Are you feeling a bit uneasy
about discussing cursing? Why? Why not?
The Write Source 2000 Teacher’s Guide (which I sent away for) suggests
grading students on a scale from 1-to-10: a "ten" for students who are
"intensely engaged"; a "one" "does not engage at all."
My son did not engage.
Boys catapulting baby carriages off roofs. Jimmy and the bloody knife on his
quilt patch, my son and his laconic disengaged homework answers—are such
behaviors symptomatic of emotional repression—are they warning signs of
potential violence?
Carol Gilligan thinks so.
She recently told the New York Times that boys are cut off emotionally; she
speaks of boys as repressing "humanity" and learning to "hurt without feeling
hurt." Pollack thinks so. He says boys are disconnected, isolated, alienated,
and trapped by a stereotypical masculinity that prevents them from expressing
their painful inner feelings. He speaks of the recent school shootings as the
tip of the iceberg.
the vast majority of boys are no more antisocial than
their female counterparts.
It is undeniable that a small subset of boys fit the Gilligan/Pollack
description of being desensitized and cut off from feelings of tenderness and
care. But the vast majority of boys are no more antisocial than their female
counterparts. Nevertheless, the boy reformers are moving ahead with their
programs to render boys less objectionable, less competitive, more emotionally
expressive—more like girls.
Carol Gilligan and her associates are looking for ways to interest boys in
gentle nurturing games. She and her associate Elizabeth Debold recently reported
finding that 3-and 4- year-old boys—"are comfortable playing house or dress up
with girls, and assuming nurturing roles in play…." They expressed their
disappointment that society rarely encourages or sustains the boys’ interest in
such activities.
"By kindergarten, peer socialization and media images kick in."
Inspired by Gilligan, gender educators around the country are now doing their
best to interest boys in dolls. This past January, my assistant Elizabeth Bowen
attended a conference at Wellesley College for Research on Women that offered a
special workshop "Dolls, Gender and Make-Believe in Early Childhood Classroom."
The Wellesley College scholars were full of ideas on how to re-socialize young
males away from competitive play and toward nurturing doll activity.
In July, I attended the 19th annual conference for the National Coalition for
Sex Equity in Education (NCSEE) (pronounced "nice-ee"). NCSEE is the
professional organization of some six hundred "sex equity experts" most of whom
work in the federal government, in state Departments of Education, and in local
schools.
Gloria Steinem once said, "We need to raise boys like we raise girls," and
the NCSEE members are working hard to put Steinem’s idea into practice. NCSEE
members consider re-socializing boys to be a matter of urgency.
I learned at the conference that many of these "equity experts" believe that
the school yard is a training ground for domestic battery. One keynote speaker
identified young male chasing behavior as conducive to future violence.
The Wellesley Center, in conjunction with the National Education Association
and the Department of Education, has produced a new teacher’s guide called Quit
It! that offers exercises on how to cope with such things as the game of tag and
other games involving chasing (p.86): "Before going outside to play, talk about
how students feel when playing a game of tag. Do they like to be chased? Do they
like to do the chasing? How does it feel to be tagged out? Get their ideas about
other ways the game might be played. Then, tell them that they are going to be
playing a different kinds of tag ‘one where nobody is ever ‘out.’"
The guide recommends and gives the rules for new, non-violent,
non-competitive version of tag called "Circle of Friends."
Once again, I find that I disagree with what the boy reformers are saying
about boys, and I very much object to what they are doing.
Frankly, I find some of these gender experts to be more than a little
aggressive themselves: with their quilts, and doll houses, and games like
"Circle of Friends."
Do they respect boys? Do they even like them?
Do they respect boys? Do they even like them?
It never occurs to the would-be-reformers of boys that their efforts to
overhaul them may be grossly unfair to them. Boys do need to be civilized. They
very much need discipline, they need to develop ethical characters: but what
they do not need is to be feminized.
It is true that boys tend to be less emotionally expressive than girls. But
that is a not a psychological or a moral failing.
Boys and girls have different styles of play. Boys, on average, are more
active. They are drawn to dynamic outdoor competitive play—with clearly defined
winners and losers. At all ages, they take more risks and sustain more injuries
than girls. Boys support a multi-million dollar industry of video and
interactive computer games: the goal of most of the games, as one father put it
is to "gain all power, and then destroy the universe." Toy manufactures have
never been able to interest many girls in such games.
For years, Mattel looked for ways to market software to girls. During the
1997 Christmas season, they finally broke through to the girl market with two
new games: "Barbie Fashion Designer" and then "Talk with me, Barbie." In the
latter game, Barbie develops a personal relationship with the girl—learning her
name and chatting about dating, careers, and playing house.
Males, young and old, are less interested than females in talking about
feelings and personal relationships. But there is no evidence that that this is
due oppressive gender stereotypes. On the contrary, the different interests and
preferences appear to be hard-wired—innate, spontaneously manifested, and
probably ineradicable.
no one has been able to show that little girls are
nicer or more virtuous than little boys.
Gilligan and other feminist talks of a female ethic, an ethic of care,
suggesting that girls are morally better, more caring than boys. But no one has
been able to show that little girls are nicer or more virtuous than little boys.
It is of course true that boys are more violent than girls. Bullying is a
problem in many schools. Boys, being stronger and generally more physically
aggressive, do most of the physical bullying, but they do not have a monopoly on
malice. Girls are proficient at what sociologists call "relational aggression."
They hurt others by shunning, excluding, spreading rumors. Almost any junior
high school girl will tell you that girls can create as much misery as boys,
especially to other girls.
I see no evidence that boys are morally inferior to girls. They are more
reticent about discussing their feelings than girls. But this is not any kind of
personality deficit. On the contrary, the reticence may actually be a virtue and
a sign of psychological health. ...
... Pollack and Gilligan assume, but never bother to demonstrate, that being
emotionally "open" is really such a good thing. That needs to be shown, not
assumed.
many psychologists are now suddenly quite skeptical
about the value of emotional expressionism
Several recent studies suggest that this popular assumption is quite simply
false. I shall be happy to tell you about this research in the question period.
But for now, it is enough to say that many psychologists are now suddenly quite
skeptical about the value of emotional expressionism.
Moral philosophers and theologians have never believed in emotional
expressionism as something to strive for. It’s not as if "Be in touch with your
feelings" was one of the Ten Commandments.
Compared to other cultures (including our own until fairly recent times),
contemporary American youth are already far too self-involved and emotionally
expressive. The reform-minded experts might even want to consider the
possibility that American children may need less, not more, self-involvement.
Not only may it be true that American boys don’t need to show more emotion, it
may also be true that American girls need to be less sentimental and
self-absorbed. Maybe all the crashing selves that Pipher talks about are selves
that are too self-preoccupied, to the unhealthy exclusion of outside interests.
Children need to be moral more than they need to be in touch with their
feelings. They need to have a strong sense of personal responsibility and clear
ideas of what is right and wrong. Children do not need feel good support groups
or 12-step programs. Above all, children don’t need to have their femininity or
masculinity "recreated" or "reconstructed," to use the boys reformers favorite
word.
Aristotle laid down what children do need almost 2500 years ago—clear
guidance on how to be moral human beings. What Aristotle advocated became the
default mode of moral education over the centuries. And it worked. It is only
very recently that many educators began to scorn it.
boys who are morally neglected have unpleasant ways of
getting themselves noticed.
A society that forsakes its traditional and proven modes of civilizing and
humanizing its male children inevitably fails them in fundamental ways. The
social costs are considerable since boys who are morally neglected have
unpleasant ways of getting themselves noticed. But the greater cost is to the
boys themselves.
Boys badly need clear, unequivocal rules. They need boundaries. They need
structure. Young men have a deep psychological need for honor. To get that need
satisfied, they need "directive moral education," explicit instruction from the
adults in their lives.
In this final section of my lecture, I want to speak of one area where I
believe boys really are in trouble. It has nothing to do with their being
pathological, insensitive, or morally shallow; it has to do with the fact that
academically, they are doing far worse than girls. This is a genuine problem.
Indeed it is a problem that could become critical.
Data from the United States Department of Education, along with several new
university studies, show unequivocally that boys are on the weak side of a
widening educational gender gap. They are less committed to school than girls,
they get lower grades, they are more likely to drop out and to be held back.
They are a full year and a half behind girls in their reading and writing
skills. They are less likely to go to college—the country’s current college
freshmen class is 56 percent female, 44 percent male. The enrollment of
African-Americans in college is 64 percent female, 36 percent male.
American boys are seriously lagging behind the girls and it keeps getting
worse. Bereft of discipline, competitive structures, and direct moral guidance
on how to compete and succeed, many American boys do behave badly. They also
fare badly academically. The therapeutic pedagogy aggravates that condition. By
emphasizing an ethic of feeling over a traditional ethic of right and wrong, by
depriving boys of the traditional, effective, time-tested classroom discipline,
modern educators are gravely harming boys.
The Daily Telegraph writer Janet Daly sums up the growing consensus in
Britain in a recent talk she gave to the Independent Women’s Forum this past
September. Referring to the "feminized curriculum" she says:
The consequences have been disastrous for boys, who it turned out, were
temperamentally much more dependent than girls on the principles of
traditional education: discipline, structure and competition.
Estelle Morris, a Labour MP who speaks for her party on the subject of
education, said, "If we do not start to address the problem young men are
facing, we have no hope." Citing many secondary schools who have "identified the
difficulties boys experience as a priority" she pointed out that some had begun
"implementing successful strategies for raising boys’ achievements and
expectations."
What are some of these strategies?
A principal of the King School was so concerned about the low performance of
his boys that he formed a boys-only remedial English class—and brought back
practices that had not been seen since the mid-sixties. Here is how one
journalist describes a King School class:
Ranks of boys in blazers face the front, giving full attention to the
young [male] teachers’ instructions. His style is uncompromising and
inspirational: "People think that boys like you won’t be able to
understand writers such as the Romantic poets. Well we’re going to prove
them wrong. Do you understand?"
According to the reporter, "The class is didactic. Teacher-fronted.
Discipline is clear-cut. If homework is not presented, it is completed in
detention. No discussion."
This past January, Stephen Byers, school standards minister and member of
British government, called for a return to traditional structured phonics for
teaching reading. He said, "a return to more structured reading lessons will
benefit both boys and girls, but the evidence show that it is boys who have been
most disadvantaged by the move away from phonics."
The British are allowing stereotypes in the textbooks—it turns out boys enjoy
and will read adventure stories with male heroes. War poetry is back.
By contrast with Britain, the American public is not aware that our boys are
languishing academically. Our government and education establishment is doing
nothing to deal with the ever-widening gender gap that threatens the future of
millions of American boys.
How are we to account for this contrast between the U.S. and Britain? What
has rendered us so benighted and them so enlightened? The short and accurate
answer is that Britain has no Carol Gilligan, no William Pollack or Mary Pipher,
no National Coalition of Sex Equity Experts, no Wellesley Center for Research on
Women, and no AAUW spreading misinformation about the nation’s girls and boys.
In this country, the alarms raised over the shortchanged girls have left no
room for concern about the academic deficits of American boys. And today, we
find the same forces that promoted the girl-crisis are promoting a boy-crisis.
Not about their serious academic deficits but about the boys’ "inner child" and
their need to be "in touch with their more feminine side."
|